A study containing four experiments provided evidence in favour of assimilation effects in retrospective duration judgments due to temporal expectations. In this study, the participants did not know in advance that they would have to reproduce the duration of a target interval. Temporal expectations were induced prior to the target interval by the repeated presentation of a visually filled interval (the expectancy interval). Both the duration of the expectancy interval and the number of presentations of that interval were varied between subjects. The experiments showed a clear assimilation effect of temporal expectations on reproduced duration, indicated by judged durations strongly resembling the duration of the ex-pectancy interval. This effect increased with the magnitude of the difference between the expectancy interval and the target interval, and with the number of repetitions of the expectancy interval. Results were discussed with reference to Helson’s adaptation-level theory.
Published in | Psychology and Behavioral Sciences (Volume 2, Issue 2) |
DOI | 10.11648/j.pbs.20130202.16 |
Page(s) | 59-65 |
Creative Commons |
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is properly cited. |
Copyright |
Copyright © The Author(s), 2013. Published by Science Publishing Group |
Assimilation Effect; Temporal Expectations; Temporal Reproduction; Retrospective Duration Judgment; Experiment
[1] | J. T. Coull, and A. C. Nobre, "Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectation with fMRI," Current Opinion in Neurobiology, vol. 18, pp. 137-144, 2008. |
[2] | L. G. Allan, "The perception of time," Perception & Psy-chophysics, vol. 26, pp. 340-354, 1979. |
[3] | A. Hellstrom, "The time-order error and its relatives: Mirrors of cognitive processes in comparing," Psychological Bulletin, vol. 97, pp. 35-61, 1985. |
[4] | K. Vierordt, Der Zeitsinn nach Versuchen [The time-sense according to experiments]. Tubingen, Germany: H. Laupp, 1868. |
[5] | H. Woodrow, "Time perception", in Handbook of experimental psychology, H. S. Langfeld (Ed.), New York: Wiley, 1951, pp. 1224-1236. |
[6] | R. Barnes, and M. R. Jones, "Expectancy, attention, and time," Cognitive Psychology, vol. 41, pp. 254-311, 2000. |
[7] | M. S. Berens, and R. E. Pastore, "Contextual relative temporal duration judgment: An investigation of sequence interruptions," Perception & Psychophysics, vol. 67, pp. 102-119, 2005. |
[8] | K. S. Ahmadi, "Effects of social influences and waiting on time judgment," Perceptual and Motor Skills, vol. 59, pp. 771-776, 1984. |
[9] | M. K. Hui, and D. K. Tse, "What to tell consumers in waits of different lengths: An integrative model of service evaluation," Journal of Marketing, vol. 60, pp. 81-90, 1996. |
[10] | F. Klapproth, "Waiting as a temporal constraint," in Limits and constraints – The study of time XIII, J. A. Parker, P. Harris, and C. Steineck (Eds.), Leiden, The Netherlands, Brill, 2010, pp. 179-198. |
[11] | H. Helson, "Fundamental problems in color vision. I. The principle governing changes in hue, saturation, and lightness of non-selective samples in chromatic illumination," Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 23, pp. 439-476, 1938. |
[12] | H. Helson, "Adaptation-level as a basis for a quantitative theory of frames of reference," Psychological Review, vol. 55, pp. 297-313, 1948. |
[13] | H. Helson, Adaptation-level theory, New York: Harper & Row, 1964. |
[14] | J. Capehart, V. J. Tempone, and J. A. Hébert, "A theory of stimulus equivalence," Psychological Review, vol. 76, pp. 405-418, 1969. |
[15] | D. R. Thomas, C. G. Jones, "Stimulus generalization as a function of the frame of reference," Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 64, pp. 77-80, 1962. |
[16] | D. R. Thomas, K. Mood, S. Morrison, and E. Wiertelak, "Peak shift revisited: A test of alternative interpretations," Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, vol. 17, pp. 130-140, 1991. |
[17] | A. Parducci, "Sequential effects in judgment," Psychological Bulletin, vol. 61, pp. 163-167, 1964. |
[18] | D. R. Thomas, "A model for adaptation-level effects on stimulus generalization," Psychological Review, vol. 100, pp. 658-673, 1993. |
[19] | L. A. Bizo, and C. V. McMahon, "Temporal generalization and peak shift in humans," Learning & Behavior, vol. 35, pp. 123-130, 2007. |
[20] | F. Klapproth, and M. Müller, "Temporal generalization under time pressure in humans," Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 61, pp. 588-600, 2008. |
[21] | F. Klapproth, and J. H. Wearden, "Why do temporal gener-alization gradients change when people make decisions as quickly as possible?" Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 64, pp. 1646-1664, 2011. |
[22] | J. Gibbon, R. M. Church, and W. H. Meck, "Scalar timing in memory," in Timing and time perception. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 423, J. Gibbon and L. Allan, Eds., 1984, pp. 52-77. |
[23] | A. Hellstrom, "Time errors are perceptual: An experimental investigation of duration and a quantitative succes-sive-comparison model," Psychological Research, vol. 39, pp. 345–388, 1977. |
[24] | A. Hellstrom, "The time-order error and its relatives: Mirrors of cognitive processes in comparing," Psychological Bulletin, vol. 97, pp. 35-61, 1984. |
[25] | W. C. Michels, and H. Helson, H. "A quantitative theory of time-order effects," American Journal of Psychology, vol. 67, pp. 327–334, 1954. |
[26] | S. W. Keele, R. Nicoletti, R. I. Ivry, R. I., and R. A. Pokorny, "Mechanisms of perceptual timing: Beat-based or inter-val-based judgments?" Psychological Research, vol. 50, pp. 251-256, 1989. |
[27] | R. Brickenkamp, D2-Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test [D2 attentional load test], Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe, 2002. |
[28] | J. H. Wearden, "Applying the scalar timing model to human time psychology: Progress and challenges," in Time and mind II: Information processing perspectives, H. Helfrich (Ed.), Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers, 2003, pp. 21-39. |
[29] | H. Eisler, "Experiments on subjective duration 1868-1975: A collection of power function exponents," Psychological Bulletin, vol. 83, pp. 1154-1171, 1976. |
[30] | H. Eisler, and A. D. Eisler, "Subjective time scaling: Influence of age, gender, and type A and type B behaviour," Chronobiologia, vol. 21, pp. 185-200, 1994. |
[31] | E. Szelag, J. Kowalska, K. Rymarczyk, and E. Pöppel, "Du-ration processing in children as determined by time repro-duction: Implications for a few seconds temporal window," Acta Psychologica, vol. 110, pp. 1-19, 2002. |
[32] | P. Ulbrich, J. Churan, M. Fink, and M. Wittmann, "Temporal reproduction: Further evidence for two processes," Acta Psychologica, vol. 125, pp. 51-65, 2007. |
APA Style
Florian Klapproth. (2013). Temporal Expectations Affect Retrospective Temporal Judgments: Evidence in Favour of Assimilation Effects. Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, 2(2), 59-65. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20130202.16
ACS Style
Florian Klapproth. Temporal Expectations Affect Retrospective Temporal Judgments: Evidence in Favour of Assimilation Effects. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 2013, 2(2), 59-65. doi: 10.11648/j.pbs.20130202.16
AMA Style
Florian Klapproth. Temporal Expectations Affect Retrospective Temporal Judgments: Evidence in Favour of Assimilation Effects. Psychol Behav Sci. 2013;2(2):59-65. doi: 10.11648/j.pbs.20130202.16
@article{10.11648/j.pbs.20130202.16, author = {Florian Klapproth}, title = {Temporal Expectations Affect Retrospective Temporal Judgments: Evidence in Favour of Assimilation Effects}, journal = {Psychology and Behavioral Sciences}, volume = {2}, number = {2}, pages = {59-65}, doi = {10.11648/j.pbs.20130202.16}, url = {https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20130202.16}, eprint = {https://article.sciencepublishinggroup.com/pdf/10.11648.j.pbs.20130202.16}, abstract = {A study containing four experiments provided evidence in favour of assimilation effects in retrospective duration judgments due to temporal expectations. In this study, the participants did not know in advance that they would have to reproduce the duration of a target interval. Temporal expectations were induced prior to the target interval by the repeated presentation of a visually filled interval (the expectancy interval). Both the duration of the expectancy interval and the number of presentations of that interval were varied between subjects. The experiments showed a clear assimilation effect of temporal expectations on reproduced duration, indicated by judged durations strongly resembling the duration of the ex-pectancy interval. This effect increased with the magnitude of the difference between the expectancy interval and the target interval, and with the number of repetitions of the expectancy interval. Results were discussed with reference to Helson’s adaptation-level theory.}, year = {2013} }
TY - JOUR T1 - Temporal Expectations Affect Retrospective Temporal Judgments: Evidence in Favour of Assimilation Effects AU - Florian Klapproth Y1 - 2013/04/02 PY - 2013 N1 - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20130202.16 DO - 10.11648/j.pbs.20130202.16 T2 - Psychology and Behavioral Sciences JF - Psychology and Behavioral Sciences JO - Psychology and Behavioral Sciences SP - 59 EP - 65 PB - Science Publishing Group SN - 2328-7845 UR - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20130202.16 AB - A study containing four experiments provided evidence in favour of assimilation effects in retrospective duration judgments due to temporal expectations. In this study, the participants did not know in advance that they would have to reproduce the duration of a target interval. Temporal expectations were induced prior to the target interval by the repeated presentation of a visually filled interval (the expectancy interval). Both the duration of the expectancy interval and the number of presentations of that interval were varied between subjects. The experiments showed a clear assimilation effect of temporal expectations on reproduced duration, indicated by judged durations strongly resembling the duration of the ex-pectancy interval. This effect increased with the magnitude of the difference between the expectancy interval and the target interval, and with the number of repetitions of the expectancy interval. Results were discussed with reference to Helson’s adaptation-level theory. VL - 2 IS - 2 ER -